Languages and Communication Systems for Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children

Languages and Communication Systems for Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children

The purpose of this resource is to describe the various language and communication tools/systems deaf and hard of hearing (DHH) children might use. This document does not address outcomes related to each area discussed. Information on outcomes can be found in the Language and Communication of Deaf and Hard of Hearing (DHH) Individuals Evidence Map.

A child’s family dynamics and language learning environment are key considerations in counseling families/care partners about language and communication planning and the desired outcomes for their child (Erbasi et al., 2017; Maddell, 2015; Moeller & Tomblin, 2015; Porter et al., 2018; Spencer & Marschark, 2010). Conversations with families/care partners about language and communication can be complex, particularly as they encounter information from different perspectives. Providing families with evidenced-based information to make the best informed decisions for their child and family is an ongoing process (Humphries et al., 2019; Lenihan & Gardiner-Walsh, 2020; Singer et al., 2020; Thompson Beckley, 2016). Some families/care partners initially explore a range of languages and communication systems and may change or combine approaches over time to adjust to their child’s and family’s needs and preferred outcomes (Ching, Scarinci, et al., 2018; DesGeorges, 2016; Knoors, 2016; National Center for Hearing Assessment and Management, 2021; Putz, 2012; Scarinci et al., 2018). Positive outcomes require that children have early and consistent access to a language, exposure to proficient language models, opportunities to communicate fully and effectively in their daily routines, and appropriate supports and services implemented with fidelity (American Speech-Language-Hearing Association [ASHA], 2013; Beal-Alvarez, 2014; Ching, Dillon, et al., 2018; Joint Committee on Infant Hearing, 2019; Moeller & Tomblin, 2015; National Association of State Directors of Special Education [NASDSE], 2018; Tomblin et al., 2015; White, 2018).

Communication Approaches

Children who are deaf and hard of hearing (DHH) use different languages, tools, systems, technologies, and strategies to communicate based on the child’s and the family/care partner’s needs and desired outcomes. Languages as well as communication tools and systems may include, but are not limited to, the following:

  • languages
    • signed languages (e.g., American Sign Language [ASL], Mexican Sign Language)
    • spoken languages (e.g., English, Spanish)
    • a combination of signed and/or spoken languages (e.g., bilingualism/multilingualism)
  • visual supports and tools
    • signing systems to support or represent spoken languages (e.g., Manually Coded English [MCE], Signing Exact English)
    • cueing systems (e.g., cued speech)
    • gestures, facial expressions, pictures, and written words
  • augmentative and alternative communication (AAC)
  • a combination of languages, supports and tools, and/or AAC (e.g., total communication, multimodal communication)

Language and communication plans require ongoing assessment of the child’s access to language in various contexts as they grow to determine if they are making expected progress and if the current services and supports continue to meet the child and family’s goals, priorities, and linguistic needs (NASDSE, 2018; Nussbaum, Waddy-Smith, & Doyle, 2012; Scarinci et al., 2018).

Languages

Spoken languages—such as English, Spanish, and Arabic—are rule-governed languages that have auditory and oral components to convey linguistic information. Children and families/care partners targeting spoken language may engage in listening and spoken language (LSL) interventions that emphasize the development, use, and understanding of spoken languages through listening. LSL goals emphasize early and consistent use of well-fit hearing technologies (e.g., hearing aids, cochlear implants, digital modulation systems) and engagement of families/care partners as the child’s primary language facilitators to enable spoken language development comparable to hearing peers by early childhood (AG Bell Academy for Listening and Spoken Language, 2007, 2017; Ching, Dillon, et al., 2018; Cole & Flexer, 2019; Wolfe, 2019, 2020).

LSL specialists provide guidance and coaching to families/care partners on auditory, speech, and language strategies and how to maintain devices. They also work with students and school staff to develop and execute plans that maximize receptive and expressive spoken language, literacy, functional listening, and access to academic programming for auditory learners (Estabrooks et al., 2020; Flexer, 2017; Houston & Bradham, 2011; NASDSE, 2018; Rosenzweig, 2017; Soman & Nevins, 2018).

Signed languages, such as ASL, are distinct, rule-governed visual languages that consist of handshapes, locations, movements, palm orientations, and nonmanual markers to convey linguistic information (Valli et al., 2011). ASL is independent from spoken English, other signed languages (e.g., British Sign Language), and MCE systems, such as Signing Exact English (Pittman et al., 2016; Rendel et al., 2018).

All spoken and signed languages have social, ethnic, and geographic variations and dialects and possess their own phonology, morphology, semantics, syntax, and pragmatics (ASHA, 2019). Signed languages have no standardized written form, although some written systems have been proposed (e.g., Sutton, n.d.). When developing literacy, children learn the written form of a spoken language through bilingual and second-language acquisition methods.

Individuals qualified to teach signed languages can provide intensive training when a signed language is not the family/care partner’s heritage language to promote consistent, fluent access to the language. Professionals qualified to assess signed language acquisition track language development to ensure that the child’s social, academic, and linguistic needs are being met (Cripps et al., 2016; Quinto-Pozos & Cooley, 2020; Quinto-Pozos et al., 2011; Simms et al., 2013). These assessments may include interdisciplinary efforts from teams including teachers of the DHH, ASL specialists, speech-language pathologists, Deaf mentors, and others.

Bimodal–bilingual refers to the development and use of language through two modalities (auditory and visual) and two languages (signed and spoken), such as ASL and English (Emmorey et al., 2005, 2008; Nussbaum, Scott, & Simms, 2012; Rinaldi & Caselli, 2014). The two languages are taught independently with equal value, with the goal of enhancing development in both languages (Davidson et al., 2014; Mitchiner et al., 2012; Petitto et al., 2001; Petitto & Kovelman, 2003; Yoshinaga-Itano, 2006). Bimodal–bilingual development is unique to each child depending on their backgrounds, goals, access to each language, and needs in their communication and learning environments (Hyde & Punch, 2011; Swanwick, 2016). Some DHH children may be bimodal–multilingual, having the ability to communicate in a combination of multiple signed and/or spoken languages (e.g., ASL, English, Spanish).

Communication Systems and Tools

Systems and tools are different from languages. The visual, manual, and/or augmentative systems and communication tools described below can be ways to visually represent, support, or supplement spoken and signed languages, but they are not true languages (Nussbaum & Scott, 2011; Scott & Dostal, 2019).

Sign Systems

Manually Coded English (MCE) refers to the various sign systems invented to represent the spoken English language visually on the hands. Unlike signed languages, which developed naturally in Deaf communities and have their own linguistic structure, MCE systems are linear and generally follow English language rules (Schick, 2011).

Signed English, Seeing Essential English (SEE1), and Signing Exact English (SEE2) are terms associated with forms of MCE. Each uses borrowed or modified ASL signs as a base and generally follows the grammar of English, with variations depending on each system. Many MCE systems include initialized signs and signs for grammatical markers or articles that do not exist in ASL to more clearly approximate English on the hands (Rendel et al., 2018).

Conceptually Accurate Signed English (CASE) (also called Pidgin Signed English [PSE]) or contact sign) combines features of ASL and English. It uses a wider variety of ASL concept signs than MCE in conjunction with English word order (Lenihan & Gardiner-Walsh, 2020; Stredler-Brown, 2010). Fingerspelling may be used in combination with any of these systems to spell out words that do not have a sign, introduce an unfamiliar sign, and/or add emphasis.

When the intent is to sign and speak at the same time, sign systems are often paired with spoken languages in approaches referred to as simultaneous communication (SimCom) or sign-supported speech (Lenihan & Gardiner-Walsh, 2020; Paul, 2009; Pittman et al., 2016; Schick, 2011). These systems are not recommended for infants and toddlers who have not yet developed language because they are not complete languages. They are sometimes used in educational settings once a child has developed a full language to facilitate communication access and English literacy (NASDSE, 2018).

Signed languages such as ASL cannot be used simultaneously with spoken English because ASL and English have distinct grammatical structures and some English words do not have an equivalent sign in ASL. For example, the grammar of ASL often includes the use of space and nonmanual markers that are incongruent with speaking English simultaneously.

Cued Speech

Cued Speech is a system of distinct handshapes and movements used in different locations near the mouth to visually depict the sounds and syllables of spoken language. It is not a language like ASL or other signed languages. It is generally used to support oral speech and language development, speechreading, and literacy by providing a visual representation for the phonology of a spoken language. For example, cued speech can help a child distinguish between sounds that look the same when spoken (e.g., /p/, /b/, and /m/). Cued speech can aid in auditory skill development when combined with early, consistent, and appropriate use of hearing technology. Cueing systems are available for approximately 60 languages. They can be used in conjunction with signed languages when families/care partners have a goal of bimodal bilingualism (LaSasso et al., 2010; National Cued Speech Association, 2019).

AAC

AAC tools and techniques may be beneficial for some children who are identified as DHH. AAC strategies can aid in spoken language development for those who are not yet using speech in a functional way and/or struggling with language concepts (Meinzen-Derr, 2018; Meinzen-Derr et al., 2019, 2021; Resource Materials and Technology Center for the Deaf/Hard of Hearing, 2018). DHH individuals who use sign language are entitled to an interpreter. At times, AAC may be a way to support communication for DHH children. Approximately 35%–50% of children who are identified as DHH also have additional diagnoses (e.g., autism, intellectual disability, visual impairment) and complex communication and/or motor needs that may benefit from the use of AAC as a primary expressive communication method (Mitchell & Karchmer, 2006; Singer et al., 2020). In such instances, these children still require similar considerations to other DHH children in selecting accessible languages and communication tools for their receptive communication development.

See ASHA’s Practice Portal page on Augmentative and Alternative Communication.

Total Communication

Total Communication encourages multimodal communication and includes a combined use of amplification, signed and/or spoken languages, MCE, Cued Speech, fingerspelling, gestures, body language, facial expressions, and AAC. However, it does not necessarily mean multimodal at all times.

The goal of Total Communication is to enhance language development using methods that are most effective for an individual child and family in a given context. In the classroom, different methods may be used at different times depending on the child’s needs and activities. A child who has been identified as deaf-blind might use tactile sign language, ProTactile communication, touch cues, tangible symbol systems, LSL, and/or AAC (National Center on Deaf-Blindness, 2021). The implementation of Total Communication may vary widely from child to child. Proponents of Total Communication consider multiple modalities to support one another in providing fuller access to language and learning (Gibbons & Szarkowski, 2019; Nussbaum & Scott, 2011; Thomas & Zwolan, 2019).

Total Communication is often mistaken for and implemented as SimCom, or sign-supported speech, in which talking and signing occur at the same time. SimCom may be used as part of a Total Communication philosophy, but is not considered synonymous with Total Communication, nor does it imply that SimCom is the desired outcome for DHH children who use multiple modalities (Mayer et al., 2016).

References

AG Bell Academy for Listening and Spoken Language. (2007). Principles of certified LSL Specialists. https://agbellacademy.org/certification/principles-of-lsl-specialists/

AG Bell Academy for Listening and Spoken Language. (2017). Certification handbook. https://agbellacademy.org/wp-content/uploads/2018/10/Certification-Handbook-1.pdf [PDF]

American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2013). Supplement to the JCIH 2007 position statement: Principles and guidelines for early intervention following confirmation that a child is deaf or hard of hearing [Position statement]. https://www.asha.org/policy/ps2013-00339/

American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2019). American Sign Language [Position statement]. https://www.asha.org/policy/ps2019-00354/

Beal-Alvarez, J. S. (2014). Deaf students’ receptive and expressive American Sign Language skills: Comparisons and relations. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 19(4), 508–529. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enu025

Ching, T. Y., Dillon, H., Leigh, G., & Cupples, L. (2018). Learning from the Longitudinal Outcomes of Children with Hearing Impairment (LOCHI) study: Summary of 5-year findings and implications. International Journal of Audiology, 57(Suppl. 2), S105–S111. https://doi.org/10.1080/14992027.2017.1385865

Ching, T. Y., Scarinci, N., Marnane, V., Sjahalam-King, J., Button, L., & Whitfield, J. (2018). Factors influencing parents’ decisions about communication choices during early education of their child with hearing loss: A qualitative study. Deafness & Education International, 20(3–4), 154–181. https://doi.org/10.1080/14643154.2018.1512393

Cole, E. B., & Flexer, C. (2019). Children with hearing loss: Developing listening and talking, birth to six. Plural.

Cripps, J. H., Cooper, S. B., Evitts, P. M., & Blackburn, J. F. (2016). Diagnosing and treating signed language disorders: A new perspective. Contemporary Issues in Communication Science and Disorders, 43(Fall), 223–237.

Davidson, K., Lillo-Martin, D., & Chen Pichler, D. (2014). Spoken English language development among native signing children with cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 19(2), 238–250. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/ent045

DesGeorges, J. (2016). Avoiding assumptions: Communication decisions made by hearing parents of deaf children. AMA Journal of Ethics, 18(4), 442–446. https://doi.org/10.1001/journalofethics.2016.18.4.sect1-1604

Emmorey, K., Borinstein, H. B., & Thompson, R. (2005). Bimodal bilingualism: Code-blending between spoken English and American Sign Language. In J. Cohen, K. T. McAlister, K. Rolstad, & J. MacSwan (Eds.), Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Bilingualism (pp. 663–673). Cascadilla Press.

Emmorey, K., Borinstein, H. B., Thompson, R., & Gollan, T. H. (2008). Bimodal bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(1), 43–61. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728907003203

Erbasi, E., Hickson, L., & Scarinci, N. (2017). Communication outcomes of children with hearing loss enrolled in programs implementing different educational approaches: A systematic review. Speech, Language and Hearing, 20(2), 102–121. https://doi.org/10.1080/2050571X.2016.1238611

Estabrooks, W., Morrison, H. M., & MacIver-Lux, K. (2020). Auditory-Verbal Therapy: An overview. In W. Estabrooks, H. M. Morrison, & K. MacIver-Lux (Eds.), Auditory-Verbal Therapy: Science, research, and practice (pp. 3–34). Plural.

Flexer, C. (2017). Start with the brain and connect the dots: Supporting children who are deaf or hard of hearing to develop literacy through listening and spoken language [White paper]. Hearing First. https://www.hearingfirst.org/m/resources/7179

Gibbons, S. M., & Szarkowski, A. (2019). One tool in the toolkit is not enough: Making the case for using multisensory approaches in aural habilitation of children with reduced hearing. Perspectives of the ASHA Special Interest Groups, 4(2), 345–355. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_PERS-SIG7-2018-0005

Houston, K. T., & Bradham, T. S. (2011). Parent engagement in audiologic habilitation: Increasing positive outcomes for children with hearing loss. The ASHA Leader, 16(8), 5–6. https://doi.org/10.1044/leader.FTR4.16082011.5

Humphries, T., Kushalnagar, P., Mathur, G., Napoli, D. J., Rathmann, C., & Smith, S. (2019). Support for parents of deaf children: Common questions and informed, evidence-based answers. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 118, 134–142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2018.12.036

Hyde, M., & Punch, R. (2011). The modes of communication used by children with cochlear implants and the role of sign in their lives. American Annals of the Deaf, 155(5), 535–549. https://www.jstor.org/stable/26235110

Joint Committee on Infant Hearing. (2019). Year 2019 position statement: Principles and guidelines for Early Hearing Detection and Intervention programs. The Journal of Early Hearing Detection and Intervention, 4(2), 1–44. https://doi.org/10.15142/fptk-b748

Knoors, H. (2016). Foundations for language development in deaf children and the consequences for communication choices. In M. Marschark & P. E. Spencer (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of deaf studies in language (pp. 19–31). Oxford University Press.

LaSasso, C. J., Crain, K. L., & Leybaert, J. (2010). Cued speech and cued language development for deaf and hard of hearing children. Plural.

Lenihan, S., & Gardiner-Walsh, S. (2020). Communication options. In S. Lenihan (Ed.), Preparing to teach, committing to learn: An introduction to educating children who are deaf/hard of hearing [eBook]. National Center for Hearing Assistance and Management. https://www.infanthearing.org/ebook-educating-children-dhh/chapters/2%20Chapter%202%202020.pdf [PDF]

Maddell, J. (2015). Counseling and support for children with hearing loss. AudiologyOnline (Article 13758). https://www.audiologyonline.com/articles/counseling-support-for-children-with-13758

Mayer, C., Marschark, M., & Spencer, P. E. (2016). Rethinking total communication: Looking back, moving forward. In M. Marschark & P. E. Spencer (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of deaf studies in language (pp. 32–44). Oxford University Press.

Meinzen-Derr, J. (2018). Augmentative and alternative communication: Optimizing language learning of children with hearing loss. The Hearing Journal, 71(3), 22–23. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.HJ.0000531217.63188.5f

Meinzen-Derr, J., Sheldon, R., Altaye, M., Lane, L., Mays, L., & Wiley, S. (2021). A technology-assisted language intervention for children who are deaf or hard of hearing: A randomized clinical trial. Pediatrics, 147(2), e2020025734. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2020-025734

Meinzen-Derr, J., Sheldon, R. M., Henry, S., Grether, S. M., Smith, L. E., Mays, L., Riddle, I., Altaye, M., & Wiley, S. (2019). Enhancing language in children who are deaf/hard-of-hearing using augmentative and alternative communication technology strategies. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 125, 23–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2019.06.015

Mitchell, R. E., & Karchmer, M. A. (2006). Demographics of deaf education: More students in more places. American Annals of the Deaf, 151(2), 95–104. https://www.jstor.org/stable/26234773

Mitchiner, J., Nussbaum, D. B., & Scott, S. (2012). The implications of bimodal bilingual approaches for children with cochlear implants (Research Brief No. 6). Visual Language and Visual Learning Science of Learning Center, Gallaudet University. https://vl2.gallaudet.edu/research-briefs/260

Moeller, M. P., & Tomblin, J. B. (2015). An introduction to the Outcomes of Children with Hearing Loss study. Ear and Hearing, 36(1), 4S–13S. https://doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0000000000000210

National Association of State Directors of Special Education. (2018). Optimizing outcomes for students who are deaf or hard of hearing: Educational service guidelines (3rd ed.). https://www.nasdse.org

National Center for Hearing Assessment and Management. (2021). Early Intervention for children who are deaf or hard of hearing: Systematic Nationwide Analysis of Program Strengths, Hurdles, Opportunities, and Trends (EI SNAPSHOT). https://www.infanthearing.org/ei-snapshot/

National Center on Deaf-Blindness. (2021). Communication basics. https://www.nationaldb.org/info-center/educational-practices/#communication-basics

National Cued Speech Association. (2019). Position statements. https://cuedspeech.org/about-us/where-we-stand/

Nussbaum, D. B., & Scott, S. M. (2011). The Cochlear Implant Education Center: Perspectives on effective educational practices. In R. Paludneviciene & I. W. Leigh (Eds.), Cochlear implants: Evolving perspectives. Gallaudet University Press.

Nussbaum, D. B., Scott, S., & Simms, L. E. (2012). The “why” and “how” of an ASL/English bimodal bilingual program. Odyssey: New Directions in Deaf Education, 13, 14–19.

Nussbaum, D., Waddy-Smith, B., & Doyle, J. (2012). Students who are deaf and hard of hearing and use sign language: Considerations and strategies for developing spoken language and literacy skills. Seminars in Speech and Language, 33(4), 310–321. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0032-1326912

Paul, P. V. (2009). Language and deafness (4th ed.). Jones & Bartlett Learning.

Petitto, L. A., Katerelos, M., Levy, B. G., Gauna, K., Tetreault, K., & Ferraro, V. (2001). Bilingual signed and spoken language acquisition from birth: Implications for the mechanisms underlying early bilingual language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 28(2), 453–496. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000901004718

Petitto, L. A., & Kovelman, I. (2003). The bilingual paradox: How signing–speaking bilingual children help us resolve bilingual issues and teach us about the brain’s mechanisms underlying all language acquisition. Learning Languages, 8(3), 5–18.

Pittman, P., Sass-Lehrer, M., & Abrams, S. (2016). Sign language, sign systems, and other visual modalities. In M. P. Moeller, D. Ertmer, & C. Stoel-Gammon (Eds.), Promoting language and literacy in children who are deaf or hard of hearing (pp. 149–180). Brookes.

Porter, A., Creed, P., Hood, M., & Ching, T. Y. (2018). Parental decision-making and deaf children: A systematic literature review. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 23(4), 295–306. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/eny019

Putz, K. (2012). The parenting journey: Raising deaf and hard of hearing children. Barefoot Publications.

Quinto-Pozos, D., & Cooley, F. (2020). A developmental disorder of signed language production in a native deaf signer of ASL. Languages, 5(4), 40–57. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5040040

Quinto-Pozos, D., Forber-Pratt, A. J., & Singleton, J. L. (2011). Do developmental communication disorders exist in the signed modality? Perspectives from professionals. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 42(4), 423–443. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2011/10-0071)

Rendel, K., Bargones, J., Blake, B., Luetke, B., & Stryker, D. S. (2018). Signing Exact English: A simultaneously spoken and signed communication option in deaf education. The Journal of Early Hearing Detection and Intervention, 3(2), 18–29. https://doi.org/10.26077/GZDH-RP64

Resource Materials and Technology Center for the Deaf/Hard of Hearing. (2018). When auditory access is not enough: When to use AAC with children who are deaf/hard of hearing [Video]. YouTube. https://youtu.be/HOLYqsta3f8

Rinaldi, P., & Caselli, M. C. (2014). Language development in a bimodal bilingual child with cochlear implant: A longitudinal study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(4), 798–809. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728913000849

Rosenzweig, E. A. (2017). Auditory Verbal Therapy: A family-centered listening and spoken language intervention for children with hearing loss and their families. Perspectives of the ASHA Special Interest Groups, 2(9), 54–65. https://doi.org/10.1044/persp2.SIG9.54

Scarinci, N., Gehrke, M., Ching, T. Y., Marnane, V., & Button, L. (2018). Factors influencing caregiver decision making to change the communication method of their child with hearing loss. Deafness & Education International, 20(3–4), 123–153. https://doi.org/10.1080/14643154.2018.1511239

Schick, B. (2011). The development of American Sign Language and Manually Coded English systems. In M. Marschark & P. E. Spencer (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and education (pp. 229–240). Oxford University Press.

Scott, J. A., & Dostal, H. M. (2019). Language development and deaf/hard of hearing children. Education Sciences, 9(2), 135. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci9020135

Simms, L., Baker, S., & Clark, M. D. (2013). The standardized Visual Communication and Sign Language Checklist for Signing Children. Sign Language Studies, 14(1), 101–124. https://www.jstor.org/stable/26191921

Singer, S. J., Cacciato, K., Kamenakis, J., & Shapiro, A. (2020). Determining language and inclusion for deaf-plus children. International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education, 13(1), 1–19.

Soman, U., & Nevins, M. E. (2018). Guiding principles and essential practices of listening and spoken language intervention in the school-age years. Topics in Language Disorders, 38(3), 202–224. https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0000000000000158

Spencer, P. E., & Marschark, M. (2010). Evidence-based practice in educating deaf and hard-of-hearing students. Oxford University Press.

Stredler-Brown, A. (2010). Communication choices and outcomes during the early years: An assessment and evidence-based approach. In M. Marschark & P. E. Spencer (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and education (pp. 292–315). Oxford University Press.

Sutton, V. (n.d.). SignWriting for sign languages. Retrieved October 1, 2022, from https://www.signwriting.org/

Swanwick, R. (2016). Deaf children’s bimodal bilingualism and education. Language Teaching, 49(1), 1–34. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0261444815000348

Thomas, E. S., & Zwolan, T. A. (2019). Communication mode and speech and language outcomes of young cochlear implant recipients: A comparison of auditory-verbal, oral communication, and total communication. Otology & Neurotology, 40(10), e975–e983. https://doi.org/10.1097/MAO.0000000000002405

Thompson Beckley, E. (2016). Refocusing on choices: Amidst renewed debate about teaching signed versus spoken language to children who are deaf, a cadre of professionals calls for emphasizing all forms of early language access. The ASHA Leader, 21(5), 44–49. https://doi.org/10.1044/leader.FTR1.21052016.44

Tomblin, J. B., Harrison, M., Ambrose, S. E., Walker, E. A., Oleson J. J., & Moeller M. P. (2015). Language outcomes in young children with mild to severe hearing loss. Ear and Hearing, 36(Suppl. 1), 76S–91S. https://doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0000000000000219

Valli, C., Lucas, C., Mulrooney, K. J., & Rankin, M. N. P. (2011). Linguistics of American Sign Language: An introduction (5th ed.). Gallaudet University Press.

White, K. R. (2018). Demographic considerations in serving children who are hard of hearing or deaf. The Journal of Early Hearing Detection and Intervention, 3(2), 14–17. https://doi.org/10.26077/K8J2-VP95

Wolfe, J. (2019). Mission probable: Age-appropriate listening and spoken language abilities for children with hearing loss [White paper]. Hearing First. https://www.hearingfirst.org/m/resources/7152

Wolfe, J. (2020). Entrain the brain: Optimize listening and spoken language outcomes of children with hearing loss [White paper]. Hearing First. https://www.hearingfirst.org/m/resources/7153

Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (2006). Early identification, communication modality, and the development of speech and spoken language skills: Patterns and considerations. In P. Spencer & M. Marschark (Eds.), Advances in the spoken language development of deaf and hard-of-hearing children (pp. 298–327). Oxford University Press.

ASHA Corporate Partners